A big theme of Flourishing Friday is the importance of maintaining a positive attitude about life and the world. I've discussed how this outlook can enhance our mental wellbeing, boost our motivation to pursue goals, foster social trust and pluralism, contribute to economic growth, and fuel efforts to advance human progress. Importantly, the benefits of a positive mindset are not exclusive to any particular group of people. It is good for everyone to approach life with a good attitude. However, a common response I receive is that holding such a positive worldview is largely a sign of privilege. Many seem to assume that our attitudes about the world reflect biases based on our own life situation. People doing well financially, enjoying good health, and living in safe environments will be more disposed to see the broader world as good, while people who are struggling financially, dealing with serious health problems, and living in more dangerous areas will be more inclined to see the broader world as bad. But is this assumption accurate? Recent research provides fascinating insights into this question.
This research builds upon the concepts of primal world beliefs (primals) that I discussed in last week's newsletter. You may recall that these primals refer to our most fundamental beliefs about the world's essential nature – whether we see it as generally good or bad, safe or dangerous, improving or declining. Last week, I explored how these beliefs positively relate to various outcomes associated with flourishing, such as job success, life satisfaction, and overall wellbeing.
This new research on the relationship between privilege and worldviews was conducted by scholars from the Positive Psychology Center at the University of Pennsylvania and the Department of Medical Social Sciences at Northwestern University. They started by exploring how widespread this belief is. Do people generally think that positive worldviews stem from privilege? To answer this, they surveyed both laypeople and psychology researchers (graduate students and professors), asking them to predict the relationship between various indicators of privilege and positive beliefs about the world. These indicators included factors such as wealth, health, gender, and neighborhood safety.
Both laypeople and researchers consistently expected substantial correlations between privileged experiences and seeing the world as safe, abundant, pleasurable, and progressing. For instance, psychology researchers predicted that living in wealthy neighborhoods would strongly correlate with believing the world is abundant. This expectation reflects a common intuition that our life circumstances significantly shape our fundamental beliefs about the world.
However, when the researchers actually tested these relationships empirically, they found a very different picture. Across multiple samples involving over 14,000 participants, they discovered that positive world beliefs (positive primals), were poor reflections of privilege. For example, the actual correlation between living in wealthy neighborhoods and seeing the world as abundant was far lower than psychology researchers assumed.
This pattern held true across various measures of privilege. Whether examining family income, childhood socioeconomic status, neighborhood safety, or even experiences with serious illness, the researchers found that these factors had surprisingly little influence on people's fundamental beliefs about the world.
The strongest relationship the researchers uncovered was between childhood trauma and the belief that the world is a dangerous place. However, even this association was modest. Similarly, individuals who had caused accidental death or serious injury to another person tended to have more negative world beliefs, but this finding came with important caveats and still showed only moderate effects.
When summarizing their findings, the researchers wrote, “While results do not preclude that some individuals' beliefs were meaningfully affected by life events, they imply that such changes are smaller or less uniform than widely believed and that knowing a person's demographic background may tell us relatively little about their beliefs (and vice versa).”
In short, having a positive outlook on the world is not simply a reflection of having led a privileged life. Conversely, experiencing hardship or disadvantage does not necessarily lead to a negative worldview.
The new findings suggest that while our basic beliefs about the world are indeed important for our wellbeing and success, as I discussed last week, they are not simply determined by our external circumstances. This opens up intriguing possibilities. If our worldviews are not inevitably shaped by our experiences of privilege or hardship, it suggests we may have more agency in cultivating positive beliefs about the world than we might have thought.
This insight could have profound implications. For those who have experienced hardship, it offers hope that a positive worldview is not out of reach. For those working to address societal challenges, it suggests that efforts to foster more positive attitudes about the world and human progress need not be hindered by assumptions about privilege.
As we navigate a cultural climate often marked by negativity and pessimism, these findings offer a compelling reason to consciously cultivate more positive beliefs about the world. Not only can this benefit our personal wellbeing and success, but it may also contribute to broader societal progress. And importantly, we now know that adopting such a perspective is not merely a reflection of privilege, but potentially a choice available to us all.
Have a great weekend!
Clay
Might there be a third factor? Could it be that positive and negative outlook are largely caused by genetic variation? That would tend to explain why people tend to have a baseline happiness that they return despite the impact of events.